He can, indeed, pull a fact from his mental library as fast
as a search engine can mine the Internet. He read Tom Clancy’s
The Hunt for Red October in one hour and 25 minutes.
Four months later, when asked, he gave the name of the Russian
radio operator in the book, referring to the page describing
the character and quoting several passages verbatim. Kim
began memorizing books at the age of 18 months, as they
were read to him. He has learned 9,000 books by heart so far.
He reads a page in eight to 10 seconds and places the memorized
book upside down on the shelf to signify that it is now
on his mental “hard drive.”
Kim’s memory extends to at least 15 interests—among
them, world and American history, sports, movies, geography,
space programs, actors and actresses, the Bible, church
history, literature, Shakespeare and classical music. He knows
all the area codes and zip codes in the U.S., together with the
television stations serving those locales. He learns the maps
in the front of phone books and can provide Yahoo-like travel
directions within any major U.S. city or between any pair
of them. He can identify hundreds of classical compositions,
tell when and where each was composed and fi rst performed,
give the name of the composer and many biographical details,
and even discuss the formal and tonal components of the music.
Most intriguing of all, he appears to be developing a new
skill in middle life. Whereas before he could merely talk about
music, for the past two years he has been learning to play it.
It is an amazing feat in light of his severe developmental
problems—characteristics shared, in varying extents, by all
savants. He walks with a sidelong gait, cannot button his
clothes, cannot manage the chores of daily life and has great
diffi culties with abstraction. Against these disabilities, his talents—
which would be extraordinary in any person—shine all
the brighter. An explanation of how Kim does what he does
would provide better insight into why certain skills, including
the ordinarily obscure skill of calendar calculating (always
associated with massive memory), occur with such regularity
among savants. Recently, when an interviewer offered that he
had been born on March 31, 1956, Kim noted, in less than a
second, that it was a Saturday on Easter weekend.
Imaging studies of Kim’s brain thus far show considerable
structural abnormality [see box on page 112]. These fi ndings
cannot yet be linked directly to any of his skills; that quest is
just beginning. Newer imaging techniques that plot the brain’s
functions—rather than just its structure—should provide more
insight, though. In the meantime, we believe it is worthwhile
to document the remarkable things that Kim can do. People
like him are not easily found, and it is useful to record their
characteristics for future research. Savantism offers a unique
window into the mind. If we cannot explain it, we cannot
claim full understanding of how the brain functions.
An Unusual Brain
kim was born on November 11, 1951 (a Sunday, he will
tell you). He had an enlarged head, on the back of which was
an encephalocele, or baseball-size “blister,” which spontaneously
resolved. But there were also other brain abnormalities,
including a malformed cerebellum. One of us (Christensen)
did the initial MRI brain scans on Kim in 1988 and has followed
his progress ever since.
|
|
Great powers of memory run through every known manifestation of savant skill. In the case of Kim Peek,
memory is itself the skill.
Kim’s brain exhibits many abnormalities, including an
absent corpus callosum. The role of that particular
abnormality in Kim’s case remains to be explained, but it
evokes a question raised by the skills of all savants:
Does brain damage stimulate compensatory
development in some other area of the brain, or does it
simply allow otherwise latent abilities to emerge?
Kim’s rote learning later developed into a form of
associative thinking, with clear evidence of creativity.
His success then helped him engage the wider world.
The authors conclude that savant skills should never be
dismissed but should be cultivated for the patient’s
intellectual and social development.
|
The cerebellar fi ndings may account for Kim’s problems
with coordination and mobility. But more striking still is the
absence of a corpus callosum, the sizable stalk of nerve tissue
that normally connects the left and right halves of the brain.
We do not know what to make of this defect, because although
it is rare, it is not always accompanied by functional
disorders. Some people have been found to lack the structure
without suffering any detectable problems at all. Yet in people
whose corpus callosum has been severed in adulthood, generally
in an effort to prevent epileptic seizures from spreading
from one hemisphere to the other, a characteristic “splitbrain”
syndrome arises in which the estranged hemispheres
begin to work almost independently of each other.
It would seem that those born without a corpus callosum
somehow develop back channels of communication between
the hemispheres. Perhaps the resulting structures allow the
two hemispheres to function, in certain respects, as one giant
hemisphere, putting functions normally rather separate under
the same roof, as it were. If so, then Kim may owe some of his
talents to this particular abnormality. In any case, the fact
that some people lacking a corpus callosum suffer no disabilities,
whereas others have savant abilities, makes its purpose
less clear than formerly thought. Neurologists joke that
its only two certain functions are to propagate seizures and
hold the brain together.
Theory guides us in one respect. Kim’s brain shows abnormalities
in the left hemisphere, a pattern found in many savants.
What is more, left hemisphere damage has been invoked
as an explanation of why males are much more likely than females to display not only savantism but also dyslexia,
stuttering, delayed speech, and autism. The proposed mechanism
has two parts: male fetuses have a higher level of circulating
testosterone, which can be toxic to developing brain
tissue; and the left hemisphere develops more slowly than the
right and therefore remains vulnerable for a longer period.
Also supporting the role of left hemisphere damage are the
many reported cases of “acquired savant syndrome,” in which
older children and adults suddenly develop savant skills after
damage to the left hemisphere.
What does all this evidence imply? One possibility is that
when the left hemisphere cannot function properly, the right
hemisphere compensates by developing new skills, perhaps by
recruiting brain tissue normally earmarked for other purposes.
Another possibility is that injury to the left hemisphere
merely unveils skills that had been latent in the right hemisphere
all along, a phenomenon some have called a release
from the “tyranny” of the dominant left hemisphere.
Kim underwent psychological testing in 1988. His overall
IQ score was 87, but the verbal and performance subtests
varied greatly, with some scores falling in the superior range
of intelligence and others in the mentally retarded range. The
psychological report concluded, therefore, that “Kim’s IQ
classifi cation is not a valid description of his intellectual ability.”
The “general intelligence” versus “multiple intelligences”
debate rages on in psychology. We believe that Kim’s case
argues for the latter point of view.
Kim’s overall diagnosis was “developmental disorder not
otherwise specifi ed,” with no diagnosis of autistic disorder.
Indeed, although autism is more commonly linked with savantism
than is any other single disorder, only about half of
all savants are autistic. In contrast with autistic people, Kim
is outgoing and quite personable. One thing that does seem
necessary for the full development of savant skills is a strong
interest in the subject matter in question.
Memory and Music
in k im’s case, all the interests began in rote memorization
but later progressed to something more. Although Kim generally
has a limited capacity for abstract or conceptual thinking—
he cannot, for example, explain many commonplace
proverbs—he does comprehend much of the material he has
committed to memory. This degree of comprehension is unusual
among savants. Down himself coined the interesting
phrase “verbal adhesion” to describe the savant’s ability to
remember huge quantities of words without comprehension.
Sarah Parker, a graduate student in psychology at the University
of Pennsylvania, in a description of a savant named Gordon
stated it more colorfully when she noted that “owning a
kiln of bricks does not make one a mason.” Kim not only
owns a large kiln of bricks, he has also become a strikingly
creative and versatile word mason within his chosen areas of
expertise.
Sometimes his answers to questions or directions are quite
concrete and literal. Once when asked by his father in a restaurant
to “lower his voice,” Kim merely slid lower into his
chair, thus lowering his voice box. In other cases, his answers
can seem quite ingenious. In one of his talks he answered a
question about Abraham Lincoln’s Gettysburg Address by
responding, “Will’s house, 227 North West Front Street. But
he stayed there only one night—he gave the speech the next
day.” Kim intended no joke, but when his questioner laughed,
he saw the point; since then, he has purposely recycled the
story with humorous intent and effect.
Yet Kim does have an undeniable power to make clever
connections. He once attended a Shakespeare festival sponsored
by a philanthropist known by the initials O.C., whose
laryngitis threatened to keep him from acknowledging a testimonial. Kim—a fan of Shakespeare, and like him, an incorrigible
punster—quipped, “O.C., can you say?”
Such creative use of material that had originally been
memorized by rote can be seen as the verbal equivalent of a
musician’s improvisation. Like the musician, Kim thinks
quickly, so quickly that it can be diffi cult to keep up with his
intricate associations. Often he seems two or three steps
ahead of his audiences in his responses.
A rather startling new dimension to Kim’s savant skills has
recently surfaced. In 2002 he met April Greenan, director of
the McKay Music Library and professor of music at the University
of Utah. With her help, he soon began to play the piano
and to enhance his discussion of compositions by playing passages
from them, demonstrating on the keyboard many of the
pieces he recalled from his massive mental library. Kim also
has remarkable long-term memory of pitch, remembering the
original pitch level of each composition.
He possesses complete knowledge of the instruments in
the traditional symphony orchestra and readily identifi es the
timbre of any instrumental passage. For example, he presented
the opening of Bedrich Smetana’s orchestral tone poem
The Moldau, by reducing the fl ute and clarinet parts to an
arpeggiated fi gure in his left hand and explaining that the
oboes and bassoons enter with the primary theme, which he
then reduced to pitches played singly and then in thirds by his
right hand (the left-hand fi gure continuing as it does in the
score). His comprehension of musical styles is demonstrated
in his ability to identify composers of pieces he had not previously
heard by assessing the piece’s musical style and deducing
who that composer might be.
A MISSING CONNECTION?
Kim Peek’s brain (bottom right) differs from typical brains
(diagram and top right) in several ways. (The scans below
are front-to-back cross sections constructed with magnetic
resonance imaging.) Kim’s brain and head are very large, each in the 99th
percentile. Most striking is the complete absence of the
corpus callosum, which normally connects the left and right
hemispheres. Missing, too, are the anterior and posterior
commissures, which also usually link the hemispheres. The
cerebellum, responsible for certain motor functions, is smaller
than usual and malformed, with fl uid occupying much of the
surrounding space; this may explain some of Kim’s diffi culties
with coordination. What role these abnormalities play in his
mental abilities is the subject of investigation.
Though Kim is still physically awkward, his manual dexterity
is increasing. When seated at the piano, he may play the
piece he wishes to discuss, sing the passage of interest or describe
the music verbally, shifting seamlessly from one mode
to another. Kim pays attention to rhythm as well, lightly tapping
the beat on his chest with his right hand or, when playing,
tapping his right foot.
Greenan, a Mozart scholar, makes these observations:
“Kim’s knowledge of music is considerable. His ability to recall
every detail of a composition he has heard—in many cases
only once and more than 40 years ago—is astonishing. The
connections he draws between and weaves through compositions,
composer’s lives, historical events, movie soundtracks
and thousands of facts stored in his database reveal enormous
intellectual capacity.” She even compares him to Mozart, who
also had an enlarged head, a fascination with numbers and
uneven social skills. She wonders whether Kim might even
learn to compose.
Life after Rain Man
i t is not surprising that Kim’s prodigious memory
caught the attention of writer Barry Morrow at a chance
meeting in 1984 and inspired him to write the screenplay for
Rain Man, whose main character, Raymond Babbitt, is a savant
played by Dustin Hoffman. The movie is purely fi ctional
and does not tell Kim’s life story, even in outline. But in one
remarkably prescient scene, Raymond instantly computes
square roots in his head, and his brother, Charlie, remarks,
“He ought to work for NASA or something.” For Kim, such a
collaboration might well happen.
NASA has proposed to make a high-resolution 3-D anatomical
model of Kim’s brain architecture. Richard Boyle,
director of the NASA BioVIS Technology Center, describes the
project as part of a larger effort to overlay and fuse image data
from as wide a range of brains as possible—and that is why
Kim’s unusual brain is of particular value. The data, both
static and functional, should enable investigators to locate
and identify changes in the brain that accompany thought and
behavior. NASA hopes that this detailed model will enable
physicians to improve their ability to interpret output from
far less capable ultrasound imaging systems, which are the
only kind that can now be carried into space and used to
monitor astronauts.
The fi lming of Rain Man and the movie’s subsequent success
proved to be a turning point in Kim’s life. Before then, he
had been reclusive, retreating to his room when company
came; afterward, the confi dence he gained from his contacts
with the fi lmmakers, together with the celebrity provided by
the movie’s success, inspired him and his father, Fran Peek, to
share Kim’s talents with many audiences. They became enthusiastic
emissaries for people with disabilities, and over the
years they have shared their story with more than 2.6 million
people.
We believe that Kim’s transformation has general applicability.
Much of what scientists know about health comes
out of the study of pathologies, and certainly much of what
will be learned about normal memory will come from the
study of unique or unusual memory. In the meantime, we
draw some practical conclusions for the care of other persons
with special needs who have some savant skill. We recommend
that family and other caregivers “train the talent,”
rather than dismissing such skills as frivolous, as a means for
the savant to connect with other people and mitigate the effects
of the disability. It is not an easy path, because disability
and limitations still require a great deal of dedication, patience
and hard work—as Kim’s father, by his example, so
convincingly demonstrates.
Further exploration of savant syndrome will provide both
scientifi c insights and stories of immense human interest. Kim
Peek provides ample evidence of both.
MORE TO E X PLORE
The Real Rain Man. Fran Peek. Harkness Publishing Consultants, 1996.
Extraordinary People: Understanding Savant Syndrome. Reprint
edition. Darold A. Treffert. iUniverse, Inc., 2000.
Islands of Genius. Darold A. Treffert and Gregory L. Wallace in
Scientifi c American, Vol. 286, No. 6, pages 76–85; June 2002.
www.savantsyndrome.com, a Web site maintained by the Wisconsin
Medical Society.